Extensive Definition
Paralanguage refers to the non-verbal
elements of communication used to
modify meaning and convey emotion. Paralanguage may be expressed
consciously or
unconsciously,
and it includes the pitch,
volume, and, in some
cases, intonation
of speech.
Sometimes the definition is restricted to vocally-produced
sounds. The study of paralanguage is known as
paralinguistics.
The term ’paralanguage’ is sometimes used as a
cover term for body
language, which is not necessarily tied to speech, and
paralinguistic phenomena in speech. The latter are phenomena that
can be observed in speech (Saussure's
parole) but that do not belong to the arbitrary conventional code
of language (Saussure's
langue).
The paralinguistic properties of speech play an
important role in human speech communication. There are no
utterances or speech signals that lack paralinguistic properties,
since speech requires the presence of a voice that can be
modulated. This voice must have some properties, and all the
properties of a voice as such are paralinguistic. However, the
distinction linguistic vs. paralinguistic applies not only to
speech but to writing
and sign
language as well, and it is not bound to any sensory modality. Even vocal
language has some paralinguistic as well as linguistic properties
that can be seen (lip reading,
McGurk
effect), and even felt, e.g. by the Tadoma method.
One can distinguish the following aspects of
speech signals and perceived utterances:
- Perspectival aspects
- Speech signals that arrive at a listener’s ears have acoustic properties that may allow listeners to localize the speaker (distance, direction). Sound localization functions in a similar way also for non-speech sounds. The perspectival aspects of lip reading are more obvious and have more drastic effects when head turning is involved.
- Organic aspects
- The speech organs of different speakers differ in size. As children grow up, their organs of speech become larger and there are differences between male and female adults. The differences concern not only size, but also proportions. They affect the pitch of the voice and to a substantial extent also the formant frequencies, which characterize the different speech sounds. The organic quality of speech has a communicative function in a restricted sense, since it is merely informative about the speaker. It will be expressed independently of the speaker’s intention.
- Expressive aspects
- The properties of the voice and the way of speaking are affected by emotions and attitudes. Typically, attitudes are expressed intentionally and emotions without intention, but attempts to fake or to hide emotions are not unusual. Expressive variation is central to paralanguage. It affects loudness, speaking rate, pitch, pitch range and, to some extent, also the formant frequencies.
- Linguistic aspects
- These aspects are the main concern of linguists. Ordinary phonetic transcriptions of utterances reflect only the linguistically informative quality. The problem of how listeners factor out the linguistically informative quality from speech signals is a topic of current research.
Some of the linguistic features of speech, in
particular of its prosody, are paralinguistic or
pre-linguistic in origin. A most fundamental and widespread
phenomenon of this kind is known as the "frequency code" (Ohala, 1984).
This code works even in communication across species. It has its
origin in the fact that the acoustic frequencies in the voice of
small vocalizers are high while they are low in the voice of large
vocalizers. This gives rise to secondary meanings such as
'harmless', 'submissive', 'unassertive', which are naturally
associated with smallness, while meanings such as 'dangerous',
'dominant', and 'assertive' are associated with largeness. In most
languages, the frequency code also serves the purpose of
distinguishing questions from statements. It is universally
reflected in expressive variation, and it is reasonable to assume
that it has phylogenetically given rise to the sexual dimorphism
that lies behind the large difference in pitch between average
female and male adults.
In text-only communication such as email, chatrooms and instant
messaging, paralinguistic elements can be displayed by emoticons, font and color choices, capitalization and the
use of non-alphabetic or abstract characters. Nonetheless,
paralanguage in written communication is limited in comparison with
face-to-face conversation, sometimes leading to
misunderstandings.
References
- Robbins, S. and Langton, N. (2001) Organizational Behaviour: Concepts, Controversies, Applications (2nd Canadian ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall
- Traunmüller, H. (2005) "Paralinguale Phänomene" (Paralinguistic phenomena), chapter 76 in: SOCIOLINGUISTICS An International Handbook of the Science of Language and Society, 2nd ed., U. Ammon, N. Dittmar, K. Mattheier, P. Trudgill (eds.), Vol. 1, pp 653-665. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin/New York.
- Ohala, J. J. (1984) An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica, 41, 1-16.
paralinguistic in Czech: Paralingvistika
paralinguistic in German:
Parasprache